Neuroscientist sees how vision runs ‘under the hood’

Neuroscientist sees how vision runs ‘under the hood’
By Brian Mezerski

            Humans open their eyes and see the world.  Vision may seem entirely uncomplicated, but there is something much deeper happening to understand this “genuine phenomenon,” according to one neuroscientist.
            David Eagleman, who works at the Baylor College of Medicine, said in a lecture to students at Elon University Monday night that most of what humans see, think and believe is created deep within the unconscious brain.  People are unaware that the process that makes vision seem easy is happening, according to Eagleman.
Neuroscientist David Eagleman answers a question
about whether different humans see the same thing.
            “The only reason vision seems effortless to us is because of the massive amounts of machinery that you have running under the hood,” Eagleman said.
            Vision is not like a camera that simply takes a picture.  Eagleman said vision is all about the internal activity in the brain, the organ located under the human “hood,” or skull.
            The brain is a key component in human vision.  One-third of it is devoted to vision, Eagleman said.  But he found, through his studies of human color interpretations, that the brain may read an image that does not match up to actual reality.
            “It makes a whole bunch of assumptions about what’s happening,” Eagleman said, “and it serves up to you a story about what color it thinks things are.”
            Eagleman said there is a phenomenon that causes different people to see these different realities.  Synesthesia is the condition in which a person’s sensory inputs are blended together, according to Eagleman.  Humans with the most common form of synesthesia see numbers, letters, days and months that trigger a color experience.
            “This is automatic and involuntary and unconscious,” Eagleman said.  “It’s not a hallucination.  Internally, there’s this experience of different colors that is self-evidently equivalent.”
            It may be difficult to distinguish those with synesthesia, a condition that affects about 4 percent of the population.  So Eagleman built an online consistency test that successfully determines who is a true synesthete.
            “We’ve been able to really revolutionize what’s been happening with understanding the behavior of synesthesia,” Eagleman said.
            Eagleman said he discovered more than 20,000 rigorously verified synesthetes through the online test.
Participants were shown a text character and then picked the best color that matches the letter or number based on their personality.  Participants later were returned to the letter again and were asked to choose a color.  Synesthetes are able to replicate their first choice, choosing the nearly identical color that they originally associated with the letter.
The study showed how vision has transitioned into a neurological analysis.
“This used to be in the realm of philosophical speculation,” Eagleman said.  “Now of days, we’ve been able to elevate this to a real scientific problem.”
The synesthesia study serves as an example of how the complexities of understanding vision can extend to the entire world.
“Any form of cross-sensory blending that you can imagine,” Eagleman said, “we have found somewhere, whether vision, hearing, taste, touch, temperature or personality.”